Skip to main content

Alcohol consumption and dependence is linked to the extent that people experience need satisfaction while drinking alcohol in two Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander communities

Abstract

Background

Unhealthy alcohol use is a key concern for Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander (‘Indigenous Australian’) communities. Due to systematic disadvantage and inter-generational trauma, Indigenous Australians may be less likely to have satisfied basic psychological needs (autonomy, competence, and relatedness). When people are need-thwarted, they may engage in compensatory behaviours to feel better in the short-term. We explore the relationship between perceived basic psychological needs satisfaction and alcohol consumption use among Indigenous Australians. Better understanding the functions that alcohol may play for some Indigenous Australian drinkers may aid communities, clinicians, and policy makers in improving programs for reducing drinking-related harms.

Methods

We performed a cross-sectional survey of Indigenous Australians (aged 16 years or older) living in two South Australian communities. Participants were eligible if they had consumed any alcohol in the past 12 months. Spearman correlations and linear regressions were used to determine if feeling more autonomous, competent, and related to others (need satisfied) while drinking, was linked to alcohol consumption and dependence.

Results

Controlling for participant demographics, reporting feeling need satisfied while drinking was linked to drinking more alcohol per day, reporting more frequent symptoms of alcohol dependence, spending more money on alcohol, and scoring higher on the AUDIT-C.

Conclusions

Unhealthy drinking may partly stem from attempts to satisfy basic psychological needs. Programs which support Indigenous Australians to meet basic psychological needs could reduce attempts to meet psychological needs through alcohol consumption.

Introduction

Alcohol is a major cause of mortality and disease worldwide [1] and can be particularly damaging to Indigenous peoples who have been colonised [2, 3]. In Australia, Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander (‘Indigenous Australian’) communities have identified risky drinking as a concern [4, 5]. While Indigenous peoples, worldwide, are not more likely to be current drinkers, those who drink are more likely to do so at risky levels [6]. But, the prevalence of risky drinking varies greatly within and between Indigenous Australian communities [7]. What drives this variability? Differences in experienced racism [8] and trauma [9] are likely linked to drinking risk, but individual motivations for drinking may also be important. Research using non-indigenous samples reveals that people are motivated to drink alcohol, in-part, to attain valued outcomes [10, 11]. Understanding the various functions that alcohol has for Indigenous Australians may help clinicians and policy makers develop tailored interventions to reduce alcohol-related harms.

Indigenous Australians and basic psychological needs

Self-determination theory (SDT) is a macro-theory of human motivation and well-being [12]. SDT posits that in order to thrive, people need to feel autonomous (free and self-determined), competent (able to be effective), and related (connected to others) [13]. When these needs are satisfied, people experience improved well-being and health [12, 14, 15]. Conversely, frustration of basic psychological needs leads to ill-being and distress [12].

In addition to supporting well-being, basic psychological need satisfaction serves to motivate many behaviours [12]. Boosting basic psychological need satisfaction has been found to motivate physical training [16], healthier eating [14, 17], better school engagement [18], and a host of other beneficial outcomes [12]. However, when people are otherwise need frustrated, they may become motivated to engage in harmful behaviours to satisfy their needs in the short-term [19]. For instance, research has shown that people experiencing basic psychological need frustration are more likely to become addicted to need-satisfying video games [20,21,22,23,24]. These need-satisfying compensatory behaviours may extend beyond video games. Alcohol consumption could be used by some people to compensate for unmet basic psychological needs.

The role of alcohol consumption in compensating for frustrated basic psychological needs may be especially relevant for Indigenous populations that have been colonised. Indeed, self-determination theory may be a useful lens through which to better understand Indigenous well-being in general [25]. Western colonisation has dispossessed Indigenous peoples of their lands, and forced them into systems which, at times, have not valued their contributions [2, 3]. Indigenous Australians can be marginalised from mainstream society and face discrimination [26], socio-economic disadvantage [27], and poorer health [27] relative to other Australians. Thus, due to societal injustice and a lack of opportunity, many Indigenous Australians may have unsatisfied basic psychological needs [28].

Drinking to compensate for thwarted needs

Work with non-indigenous populations has found people are motivated to drink, not only to cope with distress (coping motives) [29], but also to feel good (enhancement motives) [30, 31], and to regulate social functioning (to gain social rewards, and to conform to others) [32,33,34,35]. By regulating mood, and connection to others, alcohol may be used by some Indigenous Australians to meet basic psychological needs. That is, they may drink, in part, to feel more autonomous, competent, and connected to others. However, no study to date has examined whether risky drinking is linked to the extent that alcohol is perceived to satisfy basic psychological needs. If Indigenous Australians are drinking to meet needs, then risky drinking should be considered in the context of broader individual well-being.

In the current paper, we aimed to determine whether Indigenous Australians experience need satisfaction while drinking; and whether the extent to which Indigenous Australians find alcohol need-satisfying is linked to greater alcohol consumption and dependence. Drinking may satisfy the need for autonomy by making users feel disinhibited, free, and providing them with choices (what to drink, where, and with whom). Disinhibition may also help users feel connected to others (relatedness) [12, 36], especially if they are socially anxious or cannot freely talk to others when sober [35]. Finally, alcohol can temporarily enhance self-worth and confidence thus satisfying the need of competence [33]. Taken together, we hypothesise that participants who feel autonomous, connected to others, and competent while drinking, will drink more alcohol and report more frequent symptoms of alcohol dependence.

Methods

This study is part of a broader project to describe self-reported alcohol use behaviours in Indigenous Australians [37].

Research ethics

Ethical approval was obtained from the Aboriginal Health Council SA (AHCSA; Ref: 04/15/621) and from Metro South Health Human Research Ethics Committee in Queensland (Ref: HREC/16/QPAH/293).

Indigenous involvement

Australian ethical guidelines stress that Indigenous Australians should be collaborators on, and benefit from, research that draws upon their communities’ resources [38]. Study methods were designed by investigators in consultation with the Aboriginal Drug and Alcohol Council of South Australia (ADAC). Three study authors are Indigenous Australian.

Participants

Participants were Indigenous Australians, aged 16 years or older, who had consumed alcohol in the past year. They were recruited from two Indigenous Australian communities in South Australia (one urban and one remote). The urban sample was stratified to match local demographics [37]. The remote sample aimed to capture all local community members documented in the 2016 Australian Census of Population and Housing [39].

Materials

All data were collected using ‘The Grog Survey App’ (grog is a local colloquialism for alcohol) [40]. The Grog App was developed to collect data on alcohol consumption in a culturally acceptable way for Indigenous participants [41, 42]. Participants were given headphones to listen to questions in English or Pitjantjatjara (a local Indigenous Australian language). Translations were back-translated into English to verify their accuracy [43]. The App was self-administered with support from research assistants (who included Indigenous Australian health professionals).

Alcohol satisfaction of psychological needs

Basic psychological needs satisfaction and frustration scales [44] have been translated into multiple languages and adapted to many domains including: education [45], physical education [46], sport [47], romantic relationships [48], and the workplace [49]. We adapted items from the ‘Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction in Relationships’ scale [48], by replacing references to partners with references to alcohol consumption. For example the item: “When I am with my partner, I feel free to be who I am” became “When I drink, I feel free to be who I am.” Language in the scale was adapted by Indigenous Australian researchers, local language speakers, study investigators, and health professionals to ensure suitability for Indigenous Australians, and that the meaning of items was retained. We refer to this modified scale as the “Alcohol Satisfaction of Psychological Needs” (ASPN) scale. The nine items are: “When I drink, I feel free to be who I am,” “When I drink I feel like I have a say in what happens, like I can voice my opinions,” “I have to drink because friends or family want me to,” “When I drink I feel more confident,” “When I drink I feel like I’m good at things in general,” “Drinking makes me feel like I can’t do things very well,” “Drinking makes me feel close to others,” “Drinking makes me feel like part of the group” and, “When I drink, I feel lonely (even if others are around).” Items were scored on a six point Likert scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree.

AUDIT-C

The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test: Consumption questions (AUDIT-C) is a three-item screening tool used to identify individuals at risk from drinking [50,51,52]. The AUDIT-C is widely used in Indigenous Australian contexts [53, 54]. Questions were adapted by study investigators for an Indigenous Australian audience: “Some people drink grog most days, while others drink ‘once in a blue moon.’ How often have you had any grog in the last 12 months?” Responses were on a five-point scale: “never,” “once in a blue moon (less than once a month),” “sometimes (2–4 times a month),” “2–3 times a week,” “Most days or every day.” The second item was: “How much grog do you have on a typical day when you drink?” Responses were on a five-point visual scale presented in participants’ preferred type of alcohol (beer, cider, wine, spirits, or port/sherry). The response scale depicted 1–2, 3–4, 5–6, 6–9 and 10+ standard drinks (each 10 g of ethanol). The final item was paired with a visualisation of six standard drinks, shown in participants preferred alcohol type: “How often would you drink this much grog or more in 1 day (24 h)?” Responses were on the same five-point scale as for the first question ranging from “Never” to “Most days or every day.”

Alcohol consumption

The Finnish method [40, 55] was used to estimate drinking intensity (based on the last two drinking occasions) and frequency (based on the last four drinking occasions). The Finnish method has been found to be a valid and acceptable tool for measuring the alcohol consumption of Indigenous Australians [40, 41]. Drinking intensity (amount consumed per drinking occasion) was calculated by averaging the standard drinks (10 g of ethanol) of alcohol consumed over the last two occasions (in the past 12 months). Drinking occasion frequency was calculated by dividing the total number of drinking occasions by the total duration (in days) over which they were consumed [40]. An estimate of average standard drinks consumed per day was calculated by multiplying the average standard drinks per drinking occasion by the number of drinking occasions per day [40].

Alcohol dependence

Dependence questions were operationalised from ICD-11 diagnostic guidelines and worded for an Indigenous Australian audience [56]. The first question was: “Some people feel like grog is the boss of them. How often do you feel grog makes all the decisions? (so you could not stop drinking, even if you tried).” The second question was “Some people’s hands shake when they stop drinking or before their first drink of the day. In the last 12 months, how often does this happen to you?” The third question was: “Some people spend more time drinking than doing other things they need to do, like looking after family, culture or work. In the last 12 months, how often does this happen to you?” Responses were given on a five-point scale ranging from “never” to “most days or every day.” A total dependence score was calculated by summing scores across the three items.

Money spent on alcohol

Participants were asked: “On a day when you drink, how much money do you spend on grog?” Participants answered on a five-point scale: ‘$0–25,’ ‘$26–50,’ ‘$51–75,’ ‘$76–99,’ ‘$100 or more.’

Data analysis

All analyses were performed using R version 4.0.4 (2021-02-15) [57]. Responses to the ASPN were visualised for each item using the sjPlot library [58]. We performed exploratory factor analysis (EFA) [59] to determine the factor structure of the ASPN. The number of factors was selected using principal component analysis and multi-dimensional scaling as implemented in the ‘stats’ [57] and ‘corx’ [60] packages, respectively. Multi-dimensional scaling visualises item similarity. It achieves this by converting inter-item correlations to distances and then mapping items to an abstract two-dimensional space—items closer together are more similar [61]. EFA [59] was performed with the ‘psych’ [62] package. Factor scores were extracted from the EFA to be used in subsequent analyses. Spearman correlations were used to assess the link between ASPN and consumption and dependence. Multivariate linear regressions were performed using the 'stats' library [57] to determine the links between the basic psychological needs factors and alcohol consumption and dependence controlling for participant age, gender, and remoteness.

Results

Participant characteristics

We approached 799 people to take the Grog Survey App. In 24 cases (3.00%) the App was not completed. This was due to participants stopping early (n = 2), technical problems (n = 4), or because the participant was uncomfortable and did not wish to proceed (n = 8). Of those who completed the survey, one in five (22.97%) had not consumed alcohol in the past 12 months and were excluded from further analysis. The final sample of current drinkers included 597 participants. The average age of participants was 36.14 (\(SD\) = 14.74). Just over half of participants were female (n = 300; 50.25%).

Need satisfaction from consuming alcohol

Figure 1 shows how participants responded to each question about how need satisfied they feel while drinking. There was large variability in responses to questions about whether alcohol satisfied needs. Respondents tended to agree with statements about alcohol supporting needs, and disagreed with statements about alcohol thwarting needs.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Responses to the alcohol satisfaction of psychological needs questions. The x-axis displays the percentage of respondents. The area to the right of the central line displays the percentage of participants who disagreed with a given question. The area to the left shows the percentage of those who agreed

Inter-correlation matrix (ASPN)

Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlations for the ASPN items are presented in Table 1. Most items tended to have flat distributions (relative to the normal distribution; kurtosis < 3) meaning that there was large variability in responding across each item. There were moderate to strong correlations across the various needs (autonomy, competence, and relatedness). For instance, feeling like belonging to the group while drinking (relatedness satisfaction) was positively correlated with feeling pressure to drink (autonomy thwarting; \({r}_{s}\) = 0.58, \(p\) < 0.001). Feeling closer to others (relatedness satisfaction) while drinking was strongly correlated with feeling competent (competence satisfaction; \({r}_{s}\) = 0.63, \(p\) < 0.001). The three items about alcohol thwarting needs were positively correlated (albeit in most cases weakly) with items about alcohol supporting needs. This may reflect that people experience both need satisfaction and frustration while drinking.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and Pearson intercorrelations: alcohol satisfaction of psychological needs (ASPN) items

Factor analysis

Determining the number of factors

To determine how the items clustered together, we performed factor analysis. Principal component analysis was used to identify the optimal number of factors. The cumulative proportion of variance explained by each factor is presented in Fig. 2a. The first three principal components explained 67.33% of the total variance. A clear point was not observed where adding additional components added trivial amounts of variance. To aid the selection of the number of factors, multi-dimensional scaling was performed (using two dimensions) and graphed (Fig. 2b). Three major clusters of items were apparent from visual inspection of the distance between items. Feeling alone and useless clustered together, as did feeling pressure and belonging while drinking. The remaining items all referenced feeling need satisfied while drinking and formed their own larger cluster. To obtain factor loadings, we performed an EFA specifying three factors.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Scree plot (a) and Multi-Dimensional Scaling (MDS) of items (b). The scree plot describes the variance explained by each principal component. The multi-dimensional scaling plot visualises similarity between items as distances. Items closer to each other are more similar (correlated)

Exploratory factor analysis

Maximum likelihood exploratory factor analysis was performed with varimax rotation for three factors (Table 2). The first factor (‘Need satisfaction’) explained 52.26% of the total variance. This factor was comprised of all but one need satisfaction item: ‘Feeling closer,’ ‘Feeling competent,’ ‘Feeling confident,’ ‘Can voice opinions’ and ‘Feel Free.’ The second factor (‘Conformity’) explained 27.67% of variance. The items which loaded most strongly on this factor were ‘Belonging,’ and ‘Feel pressured.’ The third factor (‘Social exclusion’) accounted for 20.07% of variance. The items which loaded most strongly on this factor were both need frustration items: ‘Feel alone’ and ‘Feel useless.’

Table 2 Alcohol satisfaction of psychological needs (ASPN) factor loadings

Relationships between ASPN factors and outcome variables

ASPN factor scores were extracted for each participant, and correlated with indices of alcohol consumption and dependence. As metrics of alcohol consumption are skewed, Spearman correlations were computed (Table 3). All three factors were positively correlated with standard drinks consumed per day, money spent on alcohol, AUDIT-C score, and symptoms of alcohol dependence. Feeling more excluded (feeling alone and useless) while drinking was strongly linked to more frequent symptoms of dependence. Alcohol consumption (AUDIT-C score, standard drinks consumed per day and money spent on alcohol) was mostly strongly linked to feeling need-satisfied while drinking.

Table 3 Spearman correlations between the factor scores and indicators of drinking

Multivariate regressions

The three ASPN factors (need satisfaction, conformity, and exclusion) were included in multivariate linear regressions (OLS) which predicted the four different outcomes: standard drinks per day, dependence symptom frequency, money spent on alcohol, and AUDIT-C score (Tables 4 and 5). Each model controlled for age, gender, and remoteness. Preliminary analyses demonstrated that the model predicting standard drinks per day resulted in biased residuals. To ensure regression assumptions were met, we log-transformed the dependent variable (standard drinks per day) and re-fit this model.

Table 4 Multivariate regressions predicting standard drinks per day and dependence score with basic psychological needs from alcohol
Table 5 Multivariate regression predicting money spent on alcohol and AUDIT-C score with basic psychological needs from alcohol

Controlling for all demographic factors, the general psychological need satisfaction factor predicted consuming more standard drinks per day, more frequent symptoms of dependence, more money spent on alcohol, and higher AUDIT-C scores. For each standard deviation increase in feeling need-satisfied while drinking alcohol, the number of standard drinks consumed per day increased by an average of 61.72% [95% CI 38.77 88.45] (Table 4). Controlling for demographics, both the conformity and exclusion factors predicted more frequent dependence symptoms (Table 4). Additionally, the conformity factor predicted spending more money on alcohol (Table 5).

Discussion

We aimed to determine whether experiencing alcohol consumption as satisfying basic psychological needs (autonomy, competence, and relatedness) is linked to risky drinking and alcohol dependence among Indigenous Australians. We found that participants who reported feeling need-satisfied while drinking reported higher alcohol consumption, spent more money on alcohol, scored higher on the AUDIT-C, and reported more frequent symptoms of alcohol dependence. These relationships were observed even when controlling for participant demographics (age, gender and remoteness). While this study was cross-sectional (causal orderings and potential mediating factors could not be assessed) these patterns of results suggest that regulating basic psychological needs could be a motivating factor behind risky drinking for Indigenous Australians. If this is the case, then clinical and public health strategies which support basic psychological needs (that make clients feel self-determined, competent, and connected to others) would probably help reduce risky drinking. Moreover, clinicians and policy makers should be cautioned that some Indigenous Australians who are using alcohol to meet basic psychological needs, may need social, or other supports put in place such that restricting their drinking does not come at a cost to their well-being [19].

Needs compensation and dependence

In our sample of Indigenous Australians, participants who felt needs-satisfied while drinking, consumed more alcohol and reported more frequent symptoms of dependence. Alcohol consumption could be used by some as a ‘quick fix’ to satisfy basic psychological needs [35]. Over short spans of time, consuming alcohol may enhance mood and feelings of social connection. The short-term enhancement of basic psychological needs could make alcohol particularly rewarding and encourage future use. However, if underlying circumstances causing need deficits are not resolved, alcohol consumption could become habitual and harms from alcohol—including dependence—more likely [63].

While positive feelings were associated with drinking, feeling socially excluded (alone and useless) was also associated with higher alcohol consumption and more frequent symptoms of dependence. Having negative experiences while drinking could indicate that people use alcohol to cope with distress (coping motives) [35], or may reflect self-critical emotions resulting from excessive drinking or alcohol dependence [56]. Regardless, it seems that drinking may both satisfy and thwart basic psychological needs. People at times may drink alcohol to meet psychological needs, and at other times drink despite alcohol consumption making their mood and well-being worse. While drinking in moderation may have short-term positive psychosocial effects, these are likely to diminish and become harms as consumption increases and is sustained over longer periods of time [64].

Many Indigenous Australians may find alcohol consumption need satisfying, yet do not engage in risky drinking. As with video gaming addictions, the extent to which drinking is perceived to meet needs may not be linked to increased drinking risk if basic psychological needs (in general) are being consistently satisfied [22]. The interaction between basic psychological need satisfaction from drinking and need satisfaction in general life could further shed light on Indigenous Australian drinking risk.

Implications

The relationship between finding alcohol need satisfying and drinking, may be helpful for policy makers, communities, and clinicians to consider. If people are drinking to experience need satisfaction, restricting alcohol will also restrict need satisfaction. Thus, rather than focusing only on the removal of alcohol, community-based or clinical programs which facilitate Indigenous Australians’ need satisfaction in other areas of life may be effective in reducing risky drinking. Social activity groups may be especially helpful as they can help clients to connect with others (meeting the need for relatedness), while engaging in intrinsically motivating challenges (meeting the need for autonomy and competence). Participation in weekly social activities has been linked to less frequent alcohol use in Aboriginal Canadian youth [65]. Activity groups could include community choirs, which have been found to reduce social isolation, and improve well-being for Indigenous Australians living with chronic diseases [66]. There are also over 1000 ‘Men’s sheds’ in Australia [67]. Men’s sheds (some exclusively for Indigenous Australians) are spaces freely available to men to work together on wood-work or mechanical projects [67]. Joining a shed has been linked to increased numbers of friends, satisfaction of social needs, and a greater sense of achievement [68]. Cultural activity groups (e.g. painting, dancing, basket-weaving, or cooking) may also help participants meet basic psychological needs and build resilience [69, 70].

Aboriginal community controlled heath services have long provided holistic, culturally-informed care [71] which may support the basic psychological needs of people at risk from drinking. For example, some services run cultural activity groups to strengthen emotional health and identity [72,73,74]. Unfortunately, the effectiveness of community-led programs are often not evaluated due to a lack of funding. But Indigenous peoples from many countries have emphasised the importance of identity, and connection to both community and culture [71]. Additionally, many Indigenous and non-indigenous health professionals intuitively understand that risky drinking does not take place in a vacuum separate to other life stressors and strengths. The utility of the current research is that it offers a potential unifying framework for understanding the relationship between basic psychological needs satisfaction, and problematic alcohol use. This framework could help researchers in identifying mediating pathways which explain why clinical programs which support basic psychological needs also reduce risky drinking.

Limitations

Items for the Alcohol Psychological Need Satisfaction scale (ASPN) were derived from similar scales in other domains. However, these items have not been applied to the domain of alcohol previously. The factor structure of the introduced ASPN scale did not mirror the structure of general basic psychological needs. While basic psychological needs have a stable three factor structure, different activities may satisfy needs in different ways [12]. Previous research has found that the three needs are highly correlated with each other which may explain why a general need satisfaction factor was found [44]. The other two factors (which emphasise conformity and exclusion) could highlight the social roles alcohol may be playing in Indigenous Australian communities, which are known for collectivist kinship cultures [75, 76]. To test the stability of this domain-specific factor structure, this study should be repeated with a larger battery of items. Including general basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration items in future studies would help clarify whether need satisfaction from alcohol predicts consumption in all people, or only in those who have unsatisfied needs in other domains of life. Mental health and trauma are probably important contextual factors for understanding alcohol consumption and dependence. Perhaps people with trauma or poorer mental health are especially likely to engage in risky drinking as a strategy to meet un-met basic psychological needs. Finally, no non-indigenous participants were included in this study. The inclusion of non-indigenous participants would help determine whether these findings are specific to Indigenous Australians.

Conclusion

How Indigenous Australians feel while drinking is linked to the amount of alcohol consumed, and the frequency of symptoms of alcohol dependence. Participants who reported feeling their basic psychological needs were satisfied while drinking tended to report higher consumption and more frequent symptoms of dependence. Supporting Indigenous Australians to meet basic psychological needs in safe ways could reduce risky drinking.

Availability of data and materials

Data for this project is stored at the University of Sydney based at Drug Health Service, KGV Building, Missenden Road, Camperdown New South Wales, 2050 Australia.

References

  1. Rehm J, Mathers C, Popova S, Thavorncharoensap M, Teerawattananon Y, Patra J. Global burden of disease and injury and economic cost attributable to alcohol use and alcohol-use disorders. Lancet. 2009;373(9682):2223–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60746-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Gracey M, King M. Indigenous health part 1: determinants and disease patterns. Lancet. 2009;374(9683):65–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. King M, Smith A, Gracey M. Indigenous health part 2: the underlying causes of the health gap. Lancet. 2009;374(9683):76–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60827-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Ministerial Council on Drug Strategy. National drug strategy: Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Peoples Complementary Action Plan 2003–2009. Australian Government Department of Health and Ageing for the National Drug Strategy. 2003.

  5. Lee KK, Jagtenberg M, Ellis CM, Conigrave KM. Pressing need for more evidence to guide efforts to address substance use among young Indigenous Australians. Health Promot J Austr. 2013;24(2):87–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Hunter E, Brady M, Hall W. National recommendations for the clinical management of alcohol-related problems in Indigenous primary care settings. Canberra: Commonwealth Department of Health and Aged Care; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Conigrave JH, Lee KK, Zheng C, Wilson S, Perry J, Chikritzhs T, Slade T, Morley K, Room R, Callinan S, Hayman N. Drinking risk varies within and between Australian Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander samples: a meta-analysis to identify sources of heterogeneity. Addiction. 2020;115(10):1817–30. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.15015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Winter T, Riordan BC, Surace A, Scarf D. Association between experience of racial discrimination and hazardous alcohol use among Māori in Aotearoa New Zealand. Addiction. 2019;114(12):2241–6. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14772.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Purdie N, Dudgeon P, Walker R. Working together: Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander mental health and wellbeing principles and practice. Canberra: Commonwealth of Australia; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Cox WM, Klinger E. A motivational model of alcohol use. J Abnorm Psychol. 1988;97(2):168–80. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.97.2.168.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G, Engels R. Why do young people drink? A review of drinking motives. Clin Psychol Rev. 2005;25(7):841–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2005.06.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Self-determination theory: basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. New York: Guilford Press; 2017.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  13. Deci EL, Ryan RM. Self-determination theory: a macrotheory of human motivation, development, and health. Can Psychol. 2008;49(3):182–5. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012801.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. van der Kaap-Deeder J, Vansteenkiste M, Soenens B, Verstuyf J, Boone L, Smets J. Fostering self-endorsed motivation to change in patients with an eating disorder: the role of perceived autonomy support and psychological need satisfaction: motivation and Eating Disorders. Int J Eat Disord. 2014;47(6):585–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Weinstein N, Khabbaz F, Legate N. Enhancing need satisfaction to reduce psychological distress in Syrian refugees. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2016;84(7):645–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Verner-Filion J, Vallerand RJ. A longitudinal examination of elite youth soccer players: the role of passion and basic need satisfaction in athletes’ optimal functioning. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2018;39:20–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.07.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Boone L, Vansteenkiste M, Soenens B, Van der Kaap-Deeder J, Verstuyf J. Self-critical perfectionism and binge eating symptoms: a longitudinal test of the intervening role of psychological need frustration. J Couns Psychol. 2014;61(3):363–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036418.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Wang Y, Tian L, Scott Huebner E. Basic psychological needs satisfaction at school, behavioral school engagement, and academic achievement: longitudinal reciprocal relations among elementary school students. Contemp Educ Psychol. 2019;56:130–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.01.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Ryan RM, Deci EL. The darker and brighter sides of human existence: basic psychological needs as a unifying concept. Psychol Inq. 2000;11(4):319–38. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_03.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Ryan RM, Rigby CS, Przybylski A. The motivational pull of video games: a self-determination theory approach. Motiv Emot. 2006;30(4):344–60. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-006-9051-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Wu AMS, Lei LLM, Ku L. Psychological needs, purpose in life, and problem video game playing among Chinese young adults. Int J Psychol. 2013;48(4):583–90. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2012.658057.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Allen JJ, Anderson CA. Satisfaction and frustration of basic psychological needs in the real world and in video games predict internet gaming disorder scores and well-being. Comput Hum Behav. 2018;84:220–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.02.034.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Rigby S, Ryan RM. Glued to games: how video games draw us in and hold us spellbound: how video games draw us in and hold us spellbound. ABC-CLIO. 2011.

  24. Rigby CS, Ryan RM. Time well-spent? Motivation for entertainment media and its eudaimonic aspects through the lens of self-determination theory. In: The Routledge handbook of media use and well-being. Routledge; 2016. p. 52–66.

  25. Craven RG, Ryan RM, Mooney J, Vallerand RJ, Dillon A, Blacklock F, Magson N. Toward a positive psychology of indigenous thriving and reciprocal research partnership model. Contemp Educ Psychol. 2016;47:32–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.04.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Mellor D. Contemporary racism in Australia: The experiences of aborigines. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2003;29(4):474–86. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167202250914.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Commonwealth of Australia, Department of the Prime Minister and Cabinet. Closing the gap report 2020. 2020.

  28. Bradshaw EL, DeHaan CR, Parker P, Curren R, Duineveld J, Di Domenico SI, Ryan R. The perceived conditions for living well: positive perceptions of primary goods linked with basic psychological needs and wellness. 2019.

  29. Parrish KH, Atherton OE, Quintana A, Conger RD, Robins RW. Reciprocal relations between internalizing symptoms and frequency of alcohol use: findings from a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin youth. Psychol Addict Behav. 2016;30(2):203–8. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000138.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Cappelli C, Pike JR, Christodoulou G, Riggs NR, Warren CM, Pentz MA. The effect of sensation seeking on alcohol use among middle school students: a latent state-trait analysis. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2019;0(0):1–9.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Cooper ML. Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychol Assess. 1994;6(2):117–28. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.6.2.117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G, Engels R. Who drinks and why? A review of socio-demographic, personality, and contextual issues behind the drinking motives in young people. Addict Behav. 2006;31(10):1844–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.12.028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Neighbors C, Larimer ME, Geisner IM, Knee CR. Feeling controlled and drinking motives among college students: contingent self-esteem as a mediator. Self Identity. 2004;3(3):207–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500444000029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Marks G, Graham JW, Hansen WB. Social projection and social conformity in adolescent alcohol use: a longitudinal analysis. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1992;18(1):96–101. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167292181014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Lewis MA, Hove MC, Whiteside U, Lee CM, Kirkeby BS, Oster-Aaland L, Neighbors C, Larimer ME. Fitting in and feeling fine: conformity and coping motives as mediators of the relationship between social anxiety and problematic drinking. Psychol Addict Behav. 2008;22(1):58–67. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.22.1.58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Carey TA, MacGregor M. Understanding why youth in remote Australia use alcohol in the ways that they do. Aust J Rural Health. 2019;27(5):380–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Lee KK, Fitts MS, Conigrave JH, Zheng C, Perry J, Wilson S, Chee DA, Bond S, Weetra K, Chikritzhs TN, Slade T. Recruiting a representative sample of urban South Australian Aboriginal adults for a survey on alcohol consumption. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2020;20(1):183. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-020-01067-y.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. National Health and Medical Research Council (Australia). Ethical conduct in research with Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander peoples and communities: guidelines for researchers and stakeholders. 2018.

  39. Australian Bureau of Statistics. Census of population and housing 2006. 2016.

  40. Lee KK, Conigrave JH, Callinan S, Wilson S, Room R, Perry J, Slade T, Chikritzhs TN, Hayman N, Weatherall T, Leggat G. Asking about the last four drinking occasions on a tablet computer as a way to record alcohol consumption in Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Australians: a validation. Addict Sci Clin Pract. 2019;14(1):15. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13722-019-0148-2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Lee KK, Conigrave JH, Al Ansari M, Wilson S, Perry J, Zheng C, Freeburn B, Room R, Callinan S, Hayman N, Chikritzhs T. Acceptability and feasibility of a computer-based application to help Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Australians describe their alcohol consumption. J Ethn Subst Abuse. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1080/15332640.2019.1579144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Lee KK, Wilson S, Perry J, Room R, Callinan S, Assan R, Hayman N, Chikritzhs T, Gray D, Wilkes E, Jack P. Developing a tablet computer-based application (“App”) to measure self-reported alcohol consumption in Indigenous Australians. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2018;18(1):8. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12911-018-0583-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. World Health Organization. Process of translation and adaptation of instruments. 2009. http://www.who.int/substance_abuse/research_tools/translation/en/.

  44. Chen B, Vansteenkiste M, Beyers W, Boone L, Deci EL, Van der Kaap-Deeder J, Duriez B, Lens W, Matos L, Mouratidis A, Ryan RM. Basic psychological need satisfaction, need frustration, and need strength across four cultures. Motiv Emot. 2015;39(2):216–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-9450-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Vandenkerckhove B, Soenens B, Van der Kaap-Deeder J, Brenning K, Luyten P, Vansteenkiste M. The role of weekly need-based experiences and self-criticism in predicting weekly academic (mal) adjustment. Learn Individ Differ. 2019;69:69–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2018.11.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Haerens L, Aelterman N, Vansteenkiste M, Soenens B, Van Petegem S. Do perceived autonomy-supportive and controlling teaching relate to physical education students’ motivational experiences through unique pathways? Distinguishing between the bright and dark side of motivation. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2015;16:26–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Delrue J, Reynders B, Broek GV, Aelterman N, De Backer M, Decroos S, De Muynck GJ, Fontaine J, Fransen K, van Puyenbroeck S, Haerens L. Adopting a helicopter-perspective towards motivating and demotivating coaching: a circumplex approach. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2019;40:110–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. La Guardia JG, Ryan RM, Couchman CE, Deci EL. Within-person variation in security of attachment: a self-determination theory perspective on attachment, need fulfillment, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;79(3):367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Schultz PP, Ryan RM, Niemiec CP, Legate N, Williams GC. Mindfulness, work climate, and psychological need satisfaction in employee well-being. Mindfulness. 2015;6(5):971–85. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-014-0338-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Babor TF, Higgins-Biddle JC, Saunders JB, Monteiro MG. The alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): guidelines for use in primary care. Geneva: Department of Mental Health and Substance Abuse, World Health Organization; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Bradley KA, McDonell MB, Bush K, Kivlahan DR, Diehr P, Fihn SD. The AUDIT alcohol consumption questions: reliability, validity, and responsiveness to change in older male primary care patients. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1998;22(8):1842–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb03991.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, Fuente JRDL, Grant M. Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption-II. Addiction. 1993;88(6):791–804. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Calabria B, Clifford A, Shakeshaft AP, Conigrave KM, Simpson L, Bliss D, Allan J. Identifying Aboriginal-Specific AUDIT-C and AUDIT-3 cutoff scores for at-risk, high-risk, and likely dependent drinkers using measures of agreement with the 10-Item Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test. Addict Sci Clin Pract. 2014;9(1):17. https://doi.org/10.1186/1940-0640-9-17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. AIHW national key performance indicators database: user guide reporting period ending 30 June 2017. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. 2017. https://www.aihw.gov.au/getmedia/8137670c-809f-4bc9-ad71-1282a15af18e/nkpi-user-guide-june2017-version3.pdf.aspx.

  55. Lee KK, Conigrave JH, Wilson S, Perry J, Callinan S, Room R, Chikritzhs TN, Slade T, Hayman N, Leggat G, Conigrave KM. Short screening tools for risky drinking in Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Australians: modified AUDIT-C and a new approach. Addict Sci Clin Pract. 2019;14(1):22. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13722-019-0152-6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. World Health Organization. ICD-11 for mortality and morbidity statistics. 2018. Retrieved 22 June 2018.

  57. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing (manual). Vienna: R Core Team; 2021. https://www.R-project.org/.

  58. Lüdecke, D. sjPlot: data visualization for statistics in social science (manual). 2020. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=sjPlot.

  59. Costello AB, Osborne J. Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Explor Factor Anal. 2005;10(7):10.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Conigrave, J. corx: create and format correlation matrices (manual). 2020. https://github.com/conig/corx.

  61. Cox MAA, Cox TF. Multidimensional scaling. In: Handbook of data visualization. Berlin: Springer; 2008. p. 315–47. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-33037-0_14.

  62. Revelle W. psych: procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research (manual). 2019. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych.

  63. Gilman SE, Abraham HD. A longitudinal study of the order of onset of alcohol dependence and major depression. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2001;63(3):277–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0376-8716(00)00216-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Murphy JG, McDevitt-Murphy ME, Barnett NP. Drink and be merry? Gender, life satisfaction, and alcohol consumption among college students. Psychol Addict Behav. 2005;19(2):184–91. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.19.2.184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Rawana JS, Ames ME. Protective predictors of alcohol use trajectories among Canadian Aboriginal youth. J Youth Adolesc. 2012;41(2):229–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-011-9716-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Sun J, Buys N. Participatory community singing program to enhance quality of life and social and emotional well-being in Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Australians with chronic diseases. Int J Disabil Hum Dev. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1515/ijdhd-2012-0109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Australian Men’s Shed Association. (n.d.). What is AMSA? Australian Men’s Shed Association. https://mensshed.org/about-amsa/what-is-amsa/

  68. Taylor J, Cole R, Kynn M, Lowe J. Home away from home: health and wellbeing benefits of men’s sheds. Health Promot J Aust. 2018;29(3):236–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Burnette CE, Lesesne R, Temple C, Rodning CB. Family as the conduit to promote Indigenous women and men’s enculturation and wellness: “I wish I had learned earlier.” J Evid Based Soc Work. 2020;17(1):1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Lee KSK, Conigrave KM, Clough AR, Wallace C, Silins E, Rawles J. Evaluation of a community-driven preventive youth initiative in Arnhem Land, Northern Territory, Australia. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2008;27(1):75–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Purcell-Khodr G, Lee KK, Conigrave J, Webster E, Conigrave K. What can primary care services do to help First Nations people with unhealthy alcohol use? A systematic review: Australia, New Zealand, USA and Canada. Addict Sci Clin Pract. 2020;15(1):31. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13722-020-00204-8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Lee KSK, Dawson A, Conigrave KM. The role of an Aboriginal women’s group in meeting the high needs of clients attending outpatient alcohol and other drug treatment: Aboriginal women’s group in drug treatment. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2013;32(6):618–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. McCalman J, Tsey K, Wenitong M, Wilson A, McEwan A, James YC, Whiteside M. Indigenous men’s support groups and social and emotional wellbeing: a meta-synthesis of the evidence. Aust J Prim Health. 2010;16(2):159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Strempel P, Australian National Council on Drugs. Indigenous drug and alcohol projects: elements of best practice. Canberra: Australian National Council on Drugs; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  75. McInerney DM. Conceptual and methodological challenges in multiple goal research among remote and very remote Indigenous Australian students. Appl Psychol. 2012;61(4):634–68. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2012.00509.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Yeo SS. Bonding and attachment of Australian Aboriginal children. Child Abuse Rev J Br Assoc Study Prev Child Abuse Neglect. 2003;12(5):292–304. https://doi.org/10.1002/car.817.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) through a Project Grant (#1087192), the Centre of Research Excellence in Indigenous Health and Alcohol (#1117198) and a Practitioner Fellowship for K Conigrave (#1117582). We would like to acknowledge the communities who supported this project. We would also like to thank Michelle Fitts, David Warrior, Shane Bond, Dudley Ah Chee, Keith Weetra, Teagan Weatherall, Mustafa Al Ansari, Taleah Reynolds, Catherine Zheng, Monika Dzidowska, and Summer Loggins for their contributions.

Funding

This work was supported by the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) as part of a Project Grant ID #1087192, a Centre of Research Excellence ID #1117198, and a Practitioner Fellowship for K Conigrave ID #1117582. Funders had no role in any aspect of the study from design through to write up.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JC, EB, RR, KL, and KC conceived of the concept of the paper. KL, SW, KC and JP designed the Grog Survey App. KL and JC designed the sampling strategy. KL and JP were part of the team that collected data. JC, EB, RR, JP, KL and KC designed the basic psychological needs scale items. JC performed statistical analyses and wrote the first manuscript draft. MD, JP, SW, gave Indigenous Australian perspective on concepts and framing. EB and RR were major contributors to writing sections on self-determination theory. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to James H. Conigrave.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approvals and consent to participate were provided from two ethics committees: Aboriginal Health Council of South Australia (ACHSA: 04-15-621) and Metro South Health Human Research Ethics Committee (Queensland; HREC/16/QPAH/293).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Conigrave, J.H., Bradshaw, E.L., Conigrave, K.M. et al. Alcohol consumption and dependence is linked to the extent that people experience need satisfaction while drinking alcohol in two Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander communities. Addict Sci Clin Pract 16, 23 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13722-021-00231-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13722-021-00231-z

Keywords